Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
2 posters
Page 1 of 1
Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
News headlines this summer paint a gloomy picture of conflict across the globe. These stories suggest that group differences—whether religious, ethnic, or political—seem to divide rather than unite us. Taking a broader perspective, however, humans exhibit little inter-group conflict compared to other primates.
Most remarkably, humans are far more open to exploring relationships with strangers. When and why people are willing to befriend strangers from other groups are fundamental questions addressed in a new paper in Scientific Reports by UC Santa Barbara researchers Anne Pisor and Michael Gurven.
"Relative to other mammals, we're really tolerant of other groups, which is hard to remember sometimes when we see Dallas, Orlando, Nice and Bavaria," said Pisor, who recently completed her Ph.D. in anthropology at UC Santa Barbara and is now a postdoctoral scholar at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. "There's a lot of archaeological and ethnographic evidence of humans maintaining friendly contacts with people of different ethnicities, religions or livelihoods—strangers from 'over there' doing something different. The question is, how did these relationships get started?"
Pisor and Gurven, professor in UC Santa Barbara's Department of Anthropology and chair of the Integrative Anthropological Sciences unit, suggest that the answer revolves around a means of buffering against risks and improving access to resources—be they food, clothes, shelter, mates, or even useful information. In our evolutionary past, these limited resources were not always available in one's local community, but friendships across group boundaries could provide access. The same is true today in modern cities: we often hear about job opportunities from distant friends or acquaintances. "The same psychology affecting when we built out-group friendships in the past should be at work today," Pisor said.
To explore the role of resource access in out-group friendships, Pisor and Gurven turned to three populations of lowland Bolivian small-scale farmers. These three populations vary widely in their reliance on markets and their exposure to other ethnic and religious groups. Participants played a game in which they had the opportunity to interact with "in-group" and "out-group" strangers—that is, strangers from the same ethnic or religious group as the participant, or from a different ethnic or religious group—by giving money to them. Unlike many similar economic experiments, donors could see pictures of potential recipients, and those receiving money learned the name of the generous (or stingy) donor—in other words, "signed, sealed and delivered," Pisor joked. She intended participants to feel that a new relationship could emerge from these gifts, and participants seemed to agree: "For example, one man said, 'If I ever meet this guy, I'm going to say I know him from your game,' " Pisor said.
Those who felt they had the least gave the most
Pisor and Gurven found that participants who felt they were worse off than other members of their local community, and thus had the most to gain from out-group friendships, gave the most to out-group strangers. Additionally, participants who had lived in more places over their lifetimes also gave more to out-group strangers—though knowledge of out-group stereotypes had no effect on levels of giving. The authors suggest that the need or desire for resources, combined with direct, especially positive, exposure to out-group members, may be key to promoting trust, tolerance and possible friendship with out-group strangers.
Pisor and Gurven suggest that their approach builds on our existing understanding of between-group friendships in the social sciences: the importance of perceived opportunities for mutual gain, previously identified as a key ingredient to these friendships, is likely rooted in the between-group resource buffering crucial throughout human history and prehistory.
"Working among formerly isolated populations now experiencing rapid change provides a good opportunity to understand the conditions that help or hinder folks to work well with people of different backgrounds," Gurven noted. "From maintaining access to territory, clean water and other natural resources—collaboration across group boundaries is crucial for many indigenous populations today."
Evolutionary-minded researchers have a growing interest in studying inter-group cooperative relationships in humans, Pisor added. "We have been too preoccupied with thinking about inter-group conflict," she said. "We know less about why humans are so prone to inter-group tolerance and friendship. A better understanding of 'why' will hopefully allow us to better nurture friendships that transcend group boundaries—and maybe reduce the volume of conflict we see in the news."
Read more at: http://phys.org/news/2016-08-humans-kind.html#jCp
Most remarkably, humans are far more open to exploring relationships with strangers. When and why people are willing to befriend strangers from other groups are fundamental questions addressed in a new paper in Scientific Reports by UC Santa Barbara researchers Anne Pisor and Michael Gurven.
"Relative to other mammals, we're really tolerant of other groups, which is hard to remember sometimes when we see Dallas, Orlando, Nice and Bavaria," said Pisor, who recently completed her Ph.D. in anthropology at UC Santa Barbara and is now a postdoctoral scholar at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. "There's a lot of archaeological and ethnographic evidence of humans maintaining friendly contacts with people of different ethnicities, religions or livelihoods—strangers from 'over there' doing something different. The question is, how did these relationships get started?"
Pisor and Gurven, professor in UC Santa Barbara's Department of Anthropology and chair of the Integrative Anthropological Sciences unit, suggest that the answer revolves around a means of buffering against risks and improving access to resources—be they food, clothes, shelter, mates, or even useful information. In our evolutionary past, these limited resources were not always available in one's local community, but friendships across group boundaries could provide access. The same is true today in modern cities: we often hear about job opportunities from distant friends or acquaintances. "The same psychology affecting when we built out-group friendships in the past should be at work today," Pisor said.
To explore the role of resource access in out-group friendships, Pisor and Gurven turned to three populations of lowland Bolivian small-scale farmers. These three populations vary widely in their reliance on markets and their exposure to other ethnic and religious groups. Participants played a game in which they had the opportunity to interact with "in-group" and "out-group" strangers—that is, strangers from the same ethnic or religious group as the participant, or from a different ethnic or religious group—by giving money to them. Unlike many similar economic experiments, donors could see pictures of potential recipients, and those receiving money learned the name of the generous (or stingy) donor—in other words, "signed, sealed and delivered," Pisor joked. She intended participants to feel that a new relationship could emerge from these gifts, and participants seemed to agree: "For example, one man said, 'If I ever meet this guy, I'm going to say I know him from your game,' " Pisor said.
Those who felt they had the least gave the most
Pisor and Gurven found that participants who felt they were worse off than other members of their local community, and thus had the most to gain from out-group friendships, gave the most to out-group strangers. Additionally, participants who had lived in more places over their lifetimes also gave more to out-group strangers—though knowledge of out-group stereotypes had no effect on levels of giving. The authors suggest that the need or desire for resources, combined with direct, especially positive, exposure to out-group members, may be key to promoting trust, tolerance and possible friendship with out-group strangers.
Pisor and Gurven suggest that their approach builds on our existing understanding of between-group friendships in the social sciences: the importance of perceived opportunities for mutual gain, previously identified as a key ingredient to these friendships, is likely rooted in the between-group resource buffering crucial throughout human history and prehistory.
"Working among formerly isolated populations now experiencing rapid change provides a good opportunity to understand the conditions that help or hinder folks to work well with people of different backgrounds," Gurven noted. "From maintaining access to territory, clean water and other natural resources—collaboration across group boundaries is crucial for many indigenous populations today."
Evolutionary-minded researchers have a growing interest in studying inter-group cooperative relationships in humans, Pisor added. "We have been too preoccupied with thinking about inter-group conflict," she said. "We know less about why humans are so prone to inter-group tolerance and friendship. A better understanding of 'why' will hopefully allow us to better nurture friendships that transcend group boundaries—and maybe reduce the volume of conflict we see in the news."
Read more at: http://phys.org/news/2016-08-humans-kind.html#jCp
Guest- Guest
Re: Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
I do believe the majority of people are kind and good.
I have read of people in the poorest living conditions in India who share the very little they have with strangers, they are not and never will be materialistic, the little furniture own and the food they eat are their sole possessions, yet they are contented.
I will read this in more depth later....food for thought.
Laters.
I have read of people in the poorest living conditions in India who share the very little they have with strangers, they are not and never will be materialistic, the little furniture own and the food they eat are their sole possessions, yet they are contented.
I will read this in more depth later....food for thought.
Laters.
Syl- Forum Detective ????♀️
- Posts : 23619
Join date : 2015-11-12
Re: Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
Syl wrote:I do believe the majority of people are kind and good.
I have read of people in the poorest living conditions in India who share the very little they have with strangers, they are not and never will be materialistic, the little furniture own and the food they eat are their sole possessions, yet they are contented.
I will read this in more depth later....food for thought.
Laters.
I think there is good and bad in everyone, just in varying degrees. Even the most nastiest people in history at some point have been kind to others they like I guess.
You make a good point about India, where to people in poorer countries matreal objects will be of a lesser importance to them, where people who do struggle daily know what it means to fight daily to survive just to eat. Knowing that to share helps the community as a whole. They know such kindness is important to help that community to survive
Take care Syl
Guest- Guest
Re: Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
I have found that people with more wealth are more stingy.
eddie- King of Beards. Keeper of the Whip. Top Chef. BEES!!!!!! Mushroom muncher. Spider aficionado!
- Posts : 43129
Join date : 2013-07-28
Age : 25
Location : England
Re: Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
eddie wrote:I have found that people with more wealth are more stingy.
Agree to a certain extent, depends what background they grew up in.
Some people who I know grew up with very little and are some of the most generoeus people I know.
I have to admit religious people can be very generous as well (excluding the Catholic Church itself, not the worshippers)
Guest- Guest
Re: Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
I know some quite wealthy couples, and a couple of very wealthy couples, and they'll buy the cheapest crap to feed their kids and themselves yet will spend fortunes on their holiday destination and clothes.
It's weird.
It's weird.
eddie- King of Beards. Keeper of the Whip. Top Chef. BEES!!!!!! Mushroom muncher. Spider aficionado!
- Posts : 43129
Join date : 2013-07-28
Age : 25
Location : England
Re: Believe it or not, humans are kind, according to study
eddie wrote:I know some quite wealthy couples, and a couple of very wealthy couples, and they'll buy the cheapest crap to feed their kids and themselves yet will spend fortunes on their holiday destination and clothes.
It's weird.
That is a tad weird
Anyway have a good vening
Guest- Guest
Similar topics
» Study: Religious kids not as kind as secular peers
» People Are Hard-Wired To Be Kind And Generous, Says Study
» Ravens are so smart it’s actually kind of disconcerting, new study finds
» Dogs give food to their 'friends' in first-of-its-kind study
» Cannabis Oil ‘Significantly’ Improves the Symptoms of Crohn’s Disease in Small, First-of-its-Kind Study
» People Are Hard-Wired To Be Kind And Generous, Says Study
» Ravens are so smart it’s actually kind of disconcerting, new study finds
» Dogs give food to their 'friends' in first-of-its-kind study
» Cannabis Oil ‘Significantly’ Improves the Symptoms of Crohn’s Disease in Small, First-of-its-Kind Study
Page 1 of 1
Permissions in this forum:
You cannot reply to topics in this forum
Sat Mar 18, 2023 12:28 pm by Ben Reilly
» TOTAL MADNESS Great British Railway Journeys among shows flagged by counter terror scheme ‘for encouraging far-right sympathies
Wed Feb 22, 2023 5:14 pm by Tommy Monk
» Interesting COVID figures
Tue Feb 21, 2023 5:00 am by Tommy Monk
» HAPPY CHRISTMAS.
Sun Jan 01, 2023 7:33 pm by Tommy Monk
» The Fight Over Climate Change is Over (The Greenies Won!)
Thu Dec 15, 2022 3:59 pm by Tommy Monk
» Trump supporter murders wife, kills family dog, shoots daughter
Mon Dec 12, 2022 1:21 am by 'Wolfie
» Quill
Thu Oct 20, 2022 10:28 pm by Tommy Monk
» Algerian Woman under investigation for torture and murder of French girl, 12, whose body was found in plastic case in Paris
Thu Oct 20, 2022 10:04 pm by Tommy Monk
» Wind turbines cool down the Earth (edited with better video link)
Sun Oct 16, 2022 9:19 am by Ben Reilly
» Saying goodbye to our Queen.
Sun Sep 25, 2022 9:02 pm by Maddog
» PHEW.
Sat Sep 17, 2022 6:33 pm by Syl
» And here's some more enrichment...
Thu Sep 15, 2022 3:46 pm by Ben Reilly
» John F Kennedy Assassination
Thu Sep 15, 2022 3:40 pm by Ben Reilly
» Where is everyone lately...?
Thu Sep 15, 2022 3:33 pm by Ben Reilly
» London violence over the weekend...
Mon Sep 05, 2022 2:19 pm by Tommy Monk
» Why should anyone believe anything that Mo Farah says...!?
Wed Jul 13, 2022 1:44 am by Tommy Monk
» Liverpool Labour defends mayor role poll after turnout was only 3% and they say they will push ahead with the option that was least preferred!!!
Mon Jul 11, 2022 1:11 pm by Tommy Monk
» Labour leader Keir Stammer can't answer the simple question of whether a woman has a penis or not...
Mon Jul 11, 2022 3:58 am by Tommy Monk
» More evidence of remoaners still trying to overturn Brexit... and this is a conservative MP who should be drummed out of the party and out of parliament!
Sun Jul 10, 2022 10:50 pm by Tommy Monk
» R Kelly 30 years, Ghislaine Maxwell 20 years... but here in UK...
Fri Jul 08, 2022 5:31 pm by Original Quill